Search www.satyamag.com

Satya has ceased publication. This website is maintained for informational purposes only.

To learn more about the upcoming Special Edition of Satya and Call for Submissions, click here.

back issues

 

May 2001
Editorial: Cabbages and Weeds: The Genius of Taxonomic Hierarchy

By Catherine Clyne

 

 

One of the most compelling arguments for the extension of some sort of legal protection to animals is the case of great apes. Usually focused on chimpanzees, the argument commonly reminds us that our DNA is nearly 98 percent identical to theirs. (See Davis, p. 16) The benefit of dwelling on this point is that it is a forceful reminder that we ourselves are animals, and shames us into realizing that we treat even our nearest relatives as mere objects. Add to this the mounting evidence showing that great apes have a certain capacity for what we call rational thought: they can learn to communicate in a human language using American Sign Language, like Koko the Gorilla and Washoe the Chimpanzee, or string together a series of symbols as Kanzi the Chimpanzee does with researcher Sue Savage-Rumbaugh. Proponents argue that because these creatures demonstrate that they have something resembling an intellectual capacity equivalent to that of a human child or severely retarded adult, we have the moral imperative to confer basic rights—similar to the Universal Declaration of Human Rights—to them. In other words, they should not be killed, tortured, jailed without due process, experimented upon, eaten, or rendered homeless by human encroachment.

That great apes should—and must—live their lives unmolested by humans is without dispute here; indeed, by virtue of simply being alive every creature should lead such an autonomous existence—what’s unnatural about the current arrangement is human interference.

What is problematic about the ‘apes first’ argument is that it appeals to one of our most basic instincts: tribalism, the need to define and belong to a group of our own kind. We are impressed with apes because they are so like us, and, for this reason, they are deserving of at least honorary membership in our club of personhood. But unlike those scrambling to join a club—a school clique, fraternity, celebrity cadre, or whatever—great apes are not jockeying for inclusion; if anything, they most likely have a vague (and not unreasonable) expectation to live lives that are characteristic of their own kind.

There is a comfort in belonging to be sure, but separation, in its very nature, requires violence—something is separated from something else when one group is deemed distinct and, commonly, superior. In the club of personhood, it is we who are interested in who joins. For what is a club without a majority that is not “in”? Those within determine who is without; and only those within enjoy the specialness of membership—tough luck for everyone else. The egregious outcome of this worldview is that, as the dominating species, it is the reality that we have imposed on earth, and comprises the criteria for that slippery slope of taxonomic hierarchy on top of which we’ve placed ourselves. Such illusions allow us to separate ourselves from the rest and are the foundation of the unquestioned belief that we are superior to all other living beings and thus have the birth-right to do with them as we please.

Clubs Like Us
Years ago, when I studied German, I was charmed to learn that the distinction between the ultimate vegetable and the undesirable and inedible weed is a negative prefix: “Kraut” meaning cabbage, becomes “Unkraut”—that which is not cabbage, lowly weeds. In that part of the world, where cabbage is a national treasure, the infinite varieties of weedy nuisances are defined in relation to a vegetable that has little prominence or importance (not to mention reverence) in the rest of the world. Elsewhere such a distinction might be considered quaint, if not meaningless.

So it is with our need to divide ourselves from the rest of the animal universe, separating human from non—the ultimate vegetable from the weeds. The deeper issue is how narcissistic it is of us to measure animals in relation to our own cherished traits in the first place. Whether it’s teaching a chimp rudimentary sign language or making a dog “shake hands” or measuring rat intelligence, we are trying to stuff a square peg into a round hole. Instead of acknowledging the 98 or so percent genetic similarity that we share with chimps (never mind that we share the majority of our genetic makeup with all other animals), we separate ourselves out by the one-plus percent difference between chimps and us.

I don’t know about you, but I’ve always been rather wary of velvet ropes. You know, the red ones that stretch onto the sidewalk signaling that some sort of exclusivity’s afoot. Usually a squad of big beefy guys dressed in dark suits stand as sentinels, determining who gets in and who doesn’t, unhitching the velvet ropes for those special folks and intimidating or deriding the riff-raff deemed unfit to pass through. But what are they guarding? It’s an idea, an illusion of whatever fits the random criteria for today’s flavor of elite. Aside from, let’s say, income or looks, there’s no difference between the people on the inside or outside, except for feelings of belonging and superiority.

In the bigger picture, what does it matter that Washoe can communicate with humans? Impressive of course, but in determining who is “worthy” of not being dominated and abused by humans, rather irrelevant. Here we are like Adam in Eden, standing on our pedestal, judging the rest of creation as “like” or “not like” us, and assigning value accordingly. The bizarre novelty that a horse can stomp out the correct answer to the equation of two plus two (whether he’s coached or not) becomes more deserving of our consideration than whether that horse has the physiology to reproduce or run well. More important to us, maybe, but to the horse?

It’s Time to Think
Personally, I am fascinated with apes and always have been. Apes are cool. It is amazing how similar they are to us, their facial expressions, emotions, gestures, etc. I have admired Jane Goodall’s work with wild chimpanzees for years and it gives me goosebumps when I see videos of Washoe engaging in conversations with others. Who isn’t impressed by all of this?

Some argue that the capacity for rational thought is special, and that it does set humans apart from other animals. To pooh-pooh and minimize this distinction is to be disingenuous. However, because we’re so smart we are the dominant species, and the question of whether or not we choose to use our intelligence wisely remains to be seen.

In the 17th century, the Italian astronomer Galileo introduced his theory of heliocentrism, that the earth revolves around the sun. As a result, he was thrown out of the Roman Catholic Church. Galileo was not excommunicated because his theory questioned the existence or supremacy of God; it was that suddenly, the earth was not the center of the universe, so we were no longer the center of the universe. Suddenly our central place in the world was in question. That’s a tough idea to swallow. It took 400 years for some people to get used to the idea—just a few years ago the Pope re-communicated Galileo. After rotting in hell for all of this time, Galileo was suddenly beamed up to purgatory or heaven even. Perhaps one day, we will get used to the idea that we ourselves are animals and that we share this universe with all sorts of entities with their own right to exist without our interference; maybe then we will become more considerate and extend some of the benefits of our club to our fellow creatures.


Thanks to Karen Davis for inspiring parts of this editorial.

 


© STEALTH TECHNOLOGIES INC.
All contents are copyrighted. Click here to learn about reprinting text or images that appear on this site.