Search www.satyamag.com

Satya has ceased publication. This website is maintained for informational purposes only.

To learn more about the upcoming Special Edition of Satya and Call for Submissions, click here.

back issues

 

June 2001
Animal Economics—What Do Animals Value?

By Marc Bekoff

 

 

While many nonhuman animal beings (hereafter animals) display remarkable cognitive skills and experience the deepest and richest of emotional lives, ranging from debilitating depression to exuberant joy, none to the best of my knowledge worries about their bank account, the stock market, or what might have happened at Y2K. So, while pieces of paper and tiny bits of metal don’t drive animals to compete with one another, they do behave in ways that lead students of animal behavior to infer that many do indeed assign value to various resources, both animate and inanimate. For most animals, the currency of choice is usually food, a potential mate, or a comfortable or safe place to rest after the day’s ups and downs.

In many species, individuals compete for food. And they’ll fight harder for food of higher quality than for food that isn’t as appealing. Food’s also valued differently depending on whether an individual is sated or starving. I’ve seen coyotes stroll by an elk carcass that has plenty of meat just waiting to be consumed, only to hunt for mice or squirrels and defend small rodent meals vigorously. Dogs, as those of you who are lucky enough to share your home with a canid companion know, can be pretty picky eaters. So too can cats. Both will often turn down some food item when they’re sated, only to beg for the same food when hungry. My companion, Jethro, who joins me for meals, loves bagels and almond butter. Even if he has just eaten he’s always ready to munch with me and keep his buddies, Zeke and Cody, at bay. But he’ll take a bit more time to crunch into a lowly and boring morsel. Jethro’s also more likely to share a bone, but not a bagel, with his friends. But, they’re all dogs of few barks when they’re content, and refusing to share a warehouse of bones just isn’t part of who they are.

Other observations of animals with food indicate that they value it. Rhesus monkeys and other primates have been observed to ignore food when others are around, only to retrieve it when others leave. Rhesus monkeys will also withhold calls announcing the presence of food when others are around. Many animals also cache food. Wolves will take into account who’s around before caching and retrieving food, indicating that the food has some value. Why share if you don’t have to?

Researchers have also studied what they call “optimal foraging” and have developed numerous highfalutin and complex theories and mathematical models about what individuals ought to do in certain situations. Many animals do indeed forage optimally, maximizing gains from food energy and minimizing energetic losses due to searching for, defending, or consuming food. This reveals that they are, in their own ways, assigning value to food—in this case caloric intake.

How animals value food is a complex affair, even in birds. We found that Steller’s jays, those large blue birds with regal crests and annoying squawks, show food preferences that are influenced by such factors as the type of seeds available, where they are located, who else is around, the relative dominance of the birds, and the distance of a feeding platform to the protective cover of nearby trees. Jays made complex decisions after evaluating the total situation, and not just one variable at a time. For example, they selected an unoccupied feeder over one occupied by another jay or a squirrel, with squirrels being avoided more than other jays. Jays also usually chose the feeder further from cover possibly because it was more accessible—there were more arrival and departure routes. The more open feeder also might have allowed them to watch other animals more easily.

Another source of evidence that leads us to believe that animals have some sense of value comes from studies of courtship and mating. Dominant males of many species, including primates and wolves, will relentlessly defend a female in heat but will allow other males access to a female who isn’t reproductively active. In bower birds, females select males who have the most attractive home adorned with scraps of various colors, indicating that they’re taking into account the resources that a male possesses.

Finally, more evidence that shows that individuals make choices that may be related to some measure of value concerns preferences for individuals with whom to share food, to protect in an altercation, or to help in rearing young. In many species, kin (relatives) are chosen over non-kin, to insure that relatives’ rather than non-relatives’ genes are propagated. When this occurs, researchers say that behavior has evolved via kin selection. While kin selection isn’t necessarily a conscious process, there’s something about relatives, perhaps their odors or vocalizations, that leads them to be preferred or protected over non-kin. In some species, familiar individuals are preferred regardless of whether they’re kin. These friends are provided food or given help in rearing their young, indicating that friendship is also a valued commodity.

So, while many animals do indeed behave as if they assign differential value to different individuals and other resources, and dominance and power are associated with who has what and who has access to desired individuals and items, most animal societies that are characterized by group or pack living are so dependent on the cooperation of all individuals that competition is rarely so divisive so as to split families or to have friends harm or maim one another. Long-term research on Isle Royale by David Mech showed that pack size in wolves was regulated by social, not food-related, factors. Mech discovered that the number of wolves who could live together in a coordinated pack was governed by the number of wolves an individual could closely bond with versus the number of competing wolves an individual could tolerate. Codes of conduct, and consequentially packs, broke down when there were too many wolves.

Humans aren’t alone in assigning value to certain objects or individuals, pursuing and defending some harder than others, and occasionally even cheating and deceiving others. But it’s not typically a “dog eat dog world.” Regrettably, we often refer to animals when describing the worst of human attributes. Humans seem to be quite unique in allowing for the uncontrolled and unequal distribution between haves and have-nots (the haves possessing multiple cars and homes with the have-nots living in streets, alleyways, and homeless shelters) and in how vigorously we’ll fight to exclude others from having a reasonably decent life.

Marc Bekoff teaches biology at the University of Colorado, Boulder, and is author of Strolling with Our Kin: Speaking For and Respecting Voiceless Animals (Lantern Books, 2000) and The Smile of a Dolphin: Remarkable Accounts of Animal Emotions (Discovery Books/Crown, 2000), and is editor of Encyclopedia of Animal Rights and Animal Welfare (Greenwood, 1998). He is co-founder with Jane Goodall of Ethologists for the Ethical Treatment of Animals (www.ethologicalethics.org).

 


© STEALTH TECHNOLOGIES INC.
All contents are copyrighted. Click here to learn about reprinting text or images that appear on this site.